El bilingüismo como protección ante la demencia: Inconsistencias empíricas y nuevas propuestas metodológicas
- Noelia Calvo U. Nacional de San Juan
- Laura Manoiloff U. Nacional de Córdoba
- Edinson Muñoz Universidad de Santiago de Chile
- Marcela Contreras Universidad de Santiago de Chile
- Agustín Ibáñez Universidad Favaloro
- Adolfo M. García Universidad Favaloro
Abstract
Bilingualism and cognitive reserve: empirical inconsistencies and new methodological proposals. Cognitive decline throughout healthy or pathological aging can be slowed down by experiences which foster cognitive reserve. In this sense, some studies have suggested that bilingualism may delay the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. However, the evidence stems from retrospective approaches yielding contradictory results. The present paper addresses these findings, identifies possible lurking variables, and outlines methodological alternatives thereof. First, we characterize possible confounding factors, namely: the criteria to establish bilingualism, differences in sample design, the instruments used to examine cognitive skills, and variables known to modulate life-long cognition. Second, we propose that these limitations could be largely circumvented through experimental approaches and the use of adequate instruments to measure such variables. Moreover, future research should incorporate tasks yielding predictable patterns of contrastive performance between bilinguals and monolinguals (bilingual disadvantages in vocabulary, null effects in working memory, advantages in inhibitory control and other executive functions), and other which could offer valuable insights (e. g., proactive interference tasks). Such considerations may shed light not just on the relationship between bilingualism and cognitive reserve, but also on more general mechanisms of cognitive compensation.Downloads
References
Abutalebi, J., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., and Weekes, B. S. (2015). The neuroprotective effects of bilingualism upon the inferior parietal lobule: a structural neuroimaging study in aging Chinese bilinguals. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 33, 3-13.
Abutalebi, J., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Sheung, L. P., Green, D. W., and Weekes, B. S. (2014). Bilingualism protects anterior temporal lobe integrity in aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(9), 2126-2133.
Adi-Japha, E., Berberich-Artzi, J., and Libnawi, A. (2010). Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development, 81(5), 1356-1366.
Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., y otros (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s y Dementia, 7, 270-279.
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., y otros (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81(22), 1938-1944.
Amieva, H., Jacqmin-Gadda, H., Orgogozo, J. M., Le Carret, N., Helmer, C., Letenneur, L., y otros (2005). The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study. Brain, 128(5), 1093-1101.
Ardal, S., Donald, M. W., Meuter, R., Muldrew, S., and Luce, M. (1990). Brain responses to semantic incongruity in bilinguals. Brain and language, 39(2), 187-205.
Ardila, Alfredo (2012). Ventas y desventajas del bilingüismo. Forma y función 25(2), 99-114. Bajo, M.T., Padilla, F., and Padilla, P. (2000). Comprehension processes in simultaneous interpreting. Benjamins Translation Library, 39, 127-142. Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., and Deary, I. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959-963.
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Tang, Y., Arnold, S. E., and Wilson, R. S. (2006). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer's disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study. The Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 406-412.
Bethel, J. W., and Schenker, M. B. (2005). Acculturation and smoking patterns among Hispanics: a review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 29(2), 143-148.
Bhugra, D., and Becker, M. A. (2005). Migration, cultural bereavement and cultural identity. World Psychiatry, 4(1), 18.
Bialystok, E., Craik F. I. M., Klein., R, and Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290-303.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., and Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459-464.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., and Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859-873.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., and Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89-129.
Bialystok, E. (2011). Coordination of executive functions in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 461-468.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., and Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240-250.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Binns, M. A., Ossher, L., and Freedman, M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: Evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology, 28, 290-304.
Bloomfield, L. (1935). Language. Londres: Allen y Unwin.
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., and Contento, S. (2010). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256-269.
Brockmole, J.R, and Logie, R.H. (2013). Age-related change in visual working memory: a study of 55,753 participants aged 8-75. Frontiers in psychology, 4: 12.
Brockmole, J.R, Parra, M.A, Della Sala, S., and Logie, R.H. (2008). Do binding deficits account for age-related decline in visual working memory? Psychonomic Bulletin y Review, 15(3): 543-7.
Brown, L.A., and Brockmole, J.R. (2010). The role of attention in binding visual features in working memory: evidence from cognitive ageing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(10): 2067-79.
Bowles, R. P., and Salthouse, T. A. (2003). Assessing the age-related effects of proactive interference on working memory tasks using the Rasch model. Psychology and Aging, 18, 608-615.
Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44(1), 195-208.
Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P. F., Tozzi, F., Furberg, H., Lichtenstein, P., and Pedersen, N. L. (2006). Prevalence, heritability, and prospective risk factors for anorexia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(3), 305-312.
Burvill, P. W. (1993). A critique of current criteria for early dementia in epidemiological studies. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 8, 553-559.
Cabeza, R., and Dennis, N.A. (2013). “Frontal lobes and aging: Deterioration and compensation” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, ed., D.T., Stuss, and R.T., Knight. (New York: Oxford University Press), 628-652.
Caetano, R., Ramisetty-Mikler, S., Rodriguez, L.A. (2008). The Hispanic Americans Baseline Alcohol Survey (HABLAS): rates and predictors of alcohol abuse and dependence across Hispanic national groups. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69, 441-448.
Calvo, N., García, A. M., Manoiloff, L., e Ibáñez, A. (2015). Bilingualism and Cognitive Reserve: A Critical Overview and a Plea for Methodological Innovations. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 7, 249, 1-17, http://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00249, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4709424/.
Calvo, N., Ibáñez, A. y García, A. M. (2016). The impact of bilingualism on working memory: A null effect on the whole may not be so on the parts. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences 7, 265.
Carlson, S. M., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental science, 11(2), 282-298.
Clare, L., Whitaker, C. J., Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E., Martyr, A., Martin‐Forbes, P., y otros (2014). Bilingualism, executive control, and age at diagnosis among people with early‐stage Alzheimer's disease in Wales. Journal of Neuropsychology. 2016 Sep;10(2):163-85. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12061. Epub 2014 Nov 25.
Clark, C. M., Sheppard, L., Fillenbaum, G. G., Galasko, D., Morris, J. C., Koss, E., y otros (1999). Variability in annual Mini-Mental State Examination score in patients with probable Alzheimer disease: a clinical perspective of data from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease. Archives of Neurology, 56(7), 857-862.
Craft, G. E., Chen, A., and Nairn, A. C. (2013). Recent advances in quantitative neuroproteomics. Methods, 61(3), 186-218.
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., and Freedman, M. (2010). Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75, 1726-1729.
Crane, P. K., Gruhl, J. C., Erosheva, E. A., Gibbons, L. E., McCurry, S. M., Rhoads, K., y otros (2010). Use of spoken and written Japanese did not protect Japanese-American men from cognitive decline in late life. Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65, 654-666.
Crocco, E., Curiel, R. E., Acevedo, A., Czaja, S. J., and Loewenstein, D. A. (2014). An evaluation of deficits in semantic cueing and proactive and retroactive interference as early features of Alzheimer's disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(9), 889-897.
Codó, E. (2008). “Interviews and questionnaires” in The Blackwell Guide to Research Methods in Bilingualism and Multilingualism, eds. L. Wei and M. G. Moyer (Oxford: Blackwell), 158-176.
Cooper, P. S., Wong, A. S., Fulham, W. R., Thienel, R., Mansfield, E., Michie, P. T., and Karayanidis, F. (2015). Theta frontoparietal connectivity associated with proactive and reactive cognitive control processes. NeuroImage, 108, 354-363.
Costa, A., Hernández, M., and Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59-86. Dalrymple-Alford, J. C., MacAskill, M. R., Nakas, C. T., Livingston, L., Graham, C.,
Crucian, G. P. and Anderson, T. J. (2010). The MoCA: well-suited screen for cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 75(19), 1717-1725.
Davenport, M. H., Hogan, D. B., Eskes, G. A., Longman, R. S., and Poulin, M. J. (2012). Cerebrovascular reserve: the link between fitness and cognitive function? Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 40(3), 153-158.
Díaz-Urueta, U., Buiza-Bueno, C., y Yanguas-Lezaun, J. (2010). Reserva cognitiva: evidencias, limitaciones y líneas de investigación futura. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 45(3), 150-155.
De Bot, K., and Jaensch, C. (2015). What is special about L3 processing? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1-15.
Duñabeitia, J. A., and Carreiras, M. (2015). The bilingual advantage: Acta est fabula? Cortex, 73.
Dunn, L. M., and Dunn, L. M. (1997). Examiner’s manual for the PPVT-III Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: Form IIIA and Form IIIB. AGS.
Ebert, P. L., and Anderson, N. D. (2009). Proactive and retroactive interference in young adults, healthy older adults, and older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(01), 83-93.
Engel de Abreu, P. M. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory, 19(5), 529-537.
Epstein, J. A., Dusenbury, L., Botvin, G., and Diaz, T. (1996). Language use and initiation of alcohol use among New York City Hispanic adolescents. Journal of Child y Adolescent Substance Abuse, 5(2), 63-79.
Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., y otros (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 3017-3022.
Fairburn, C. G., and Beglin, S. J. (1994). Assessment of eating disorders: Interview or self‐report questionnaire? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16(4), 363-370.
Feher, E. P., Mahurin, R. K., Doody, R. S., Cooke, N., Sims, J., and Pirozzolo, F. J. (1992). Establishing the limits of the Mini-Mental State: Examination of 'subtests'. Archives of Neurology, 49(1), 87-92.
Feng, X. (2009). Working memory and bilingualism: An investigation of executive control and processing speed. ProQuest.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198.
Fuller-Thomson, E., and Kuh, D. (2014). The healthy migrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 52, 128-130.
García, A. M. (2014b). Neurocognitive determinants of performance variability among world-language users. Journal of World Languages, 1(1), 60-77.
García, A. M., Manoiloff, L. y Wagner, M. (2016). Concepciones del bilingüismo y evaluación de la competencia bilingüe. En Adolfo M. García y Sonia Suárez Cepeda (eds.), Mente bilingüe: abordajes psicolingüísticos y cognitivistas (pp. 17-49). Córdoba: Comunicarte.
García-Pentón, L., Fernández García, Y., Costello, B., Duñabeitia, J. A., and Carreiras, M. (2015). The neuroanatomy of bilingualism: How to turn a hazy view into the full picture. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 1-25. doi:10.1080/23273798.2015.1068944.
Gelfo, F., De Bartolo, P., Giovine, A., Petrosini, L., and Leggio, M. G. (2009). Layer and regional effects of environmental enrichment on the pyramidal neuron morphology of the rat. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91(4), 353-365.
Geller, G., and Thomas, C. D. (1999). A review of eating disorders in immigrant women: Possible evidence for a culture-change model. Eating Disorders, 7(4), 279-297.
Ghidoni, R., Paterlini, A., Albertini, V., Binetti, G., and Benussi, L. (2012). Losing protein in the brain: the case of progranulin. Brain research, 1476: 172-82.
Gómez-Ruiz, M.I. (2010). “Bilingüismo y cerebro: Mito y realidad”. Neurología 25(7):443-
Grady, C. L., Luk, G., Craik, F. I., and Bialystok, E. (2015). Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia, 66, 170-181.
Green, M. and Rogers, P. (1998). Impairments in working memory associated with spontaneous dieting behaviour. Psychological Medicine, 28, 1063-1070.
Gleichgerrcht, E., Roca, M., Manes, F., and Torralva, T. (2011). Comparing the clinical usefulness of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO) Frontal Screening (IFS) and the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) in frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(9), 997-1004.
Gollan, T. H., Salmon, D. P., Montoya, R. I., and Galasko, D. R. (2011). Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia, 49(14), 3826-3830.
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., and Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong Bilingualism Maintains Neural Efficiency for Cognitive Control in Aging. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(2), 387-396.
Grober, E., Hall, C. B., Lipton, R. B., Zonderman, A. B., Resnick, S. M., and Kawas, C. (2008). Memory impairment, executive dysfunction, and intellectual decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(2), 266-278.
Grosjean, F. (1994). “Individual bilingualism” in The Encyclopaedia of Language and Linguistics , ed. R. E. Asher (Oxford: Pergamon Press), 1656-1660.
Henn, C., Loffelholz, K., and Klein, J. (1998) Stimulatory and inhibitory effects of ethanol on hippocampal acetylcholine release. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Archive Pharmacology, 357, 640-647.
Hindle, J. V., Martin-Forbes, P. A., Bastable, A. J. M., Pye, K. L., Martyr, A., Whitaker, C., y otros (2015). Cognitive Reserve in Parkinson’s Disease: The effects of Welsh-English bilingualism on executive function. Parkinson’s Disease, 2015, 943572.
Hogervorst, E., Huppert, F., Matthews, F. E., and Brayne, C. (2008). Thyroid function and cognitive decline in the MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(7), 1013-1022.
Hulstijn, J. H. (2010). “Measuring second language proficiency”, in Experimental Methods in Language Acquisition Research, eds. E. Blom and S. Unsworth (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 185-199.
Hulstijn, J. H. (2012). The construct of language proficiency in the study of bilingualism from a cognitive perspective. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(2), 422-433.
Hoops, S., Nazem, S., Siderowf, A. D., Duda, J. E., Xie, S. X., Stern, M. B., and Weintraub, D. (2009). Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 73(21), 1738-1745.
Kadam, P., and Bhalerao, S. (2010). Sample size calculation. International Journal of Ayurveda Research, 1(1), 55-57.
Karama, S., Ducharme, S., Corley, J., Chouinard-Decorte, F., Starr, J. M., Wardlaw, J. M. y otros (2015). Cigarette smoking and thinning of the brain’s cortex. Molecular Psychiatry.
Karch, C.M., Cruchaga, C., Goate, A.M. (2014). Alzheimer's Disease Genetics: From the Bench to the Clinic. Neuron, 83(1): 11-26.
Kave, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., and Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2008). Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23, 70-78.
Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., and Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171-174.
Kim, S. Y., and Caine, E. D. (2014). Utility and limits of the mini mental state examination in evaluating consent capacity in Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatric Services.
Kovacs, G.G., Botond, G., and Budka, H. (2010). Protein coding of neurodegenerative dementias: the neuropathological basis of biomarker diagnostics. Acta Neuropathologica, 119(4): 389-408.
Kowoll, M. E., Degen, C., Gladis, S., and Schröder, J. (2015). Neuropsychological profiles and verbal abilities in lifelong bilinguals with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s disease, 45(4), 1257-1268.
Kousaie, S., and Phillips, N. A. (2012). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 65, 356-369.
Lawton, D. M., Gasquoine, P. G., and Weimer, A. A. (2015). Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans. Cortex, 66, 141-145.
Little, T. D. (2013). The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods in Psychology, volume 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lustig, C., May, C. P., and Hasher, L. (2001). Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 130, 199-207.
Lustig, C., and Hasher, L. (2002). Working memory span: The effect of prior learning. American Journal of Psychology, 115(1), 89-101.
Mackey, W. F. (1968). “The description of bilingualism” in Readings in the Sociology of Language, eds J. Fishma (La Haya: Mouton), pp. 554-584.
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., Mizrahi, E., Kania, U., and Cordes, A. K. (2013). Multilingual Stroop performance: Effects of trilingualism and proficiency on inhibitory control. International Journal of Multilingualism, 10(1), 82-104.
Mattis, S. (1998). Dementia Rating Scale: Professional Manual. Florida: Odessa. McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., and Stadlan, E. M., (1984). Clinical-diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services task-force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939-944.
Meara, P. (1994). LLEX: Lingua Vocabulary Test v. 1.4. Swansea: Center for Applied Linguistics, University of Wales.
Michon, M. (2016). Consecuencias del bilingüismo sobre la cognición. En Adolfo M. García y Sonia Suárez Cepeda (eds.), Mente bilingüe: abordajes psicolingüísticos y cognitivistas (pp. 145-142). Córdoba: Comunicarte.
Mohs, R. C., Rosen, W. G., and Davis K. L. (1983). The Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale: an instrument for assessing treatment efficacy. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 19, 448-450.
Mohs, R. C., Cohen L. (1988). Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS). Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 627-628.
Mohs, R.C. (1994). Administration and Scoring Manual for the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale, revised Edition. New York: The Mount Sinai School of Medicine.
Morales, J., Calvo, A., and Bialystok, E. (2013a). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 187-202.
Morales, J., Gómez-Ariza, C., and Bajo T, M. (2013b). Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 531-546. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.807812
Namazi, M., and Thordardottir, E.T. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage, in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 597-616.
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., and Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695-699.
Neville, H. J., Mills, D. L., and Lawson, D. S. (1992). Fractionating language: Different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. Cerebral Cortex, 2(3), 244-258.
Neville, H. J., Coffey, S. A., Lawson, D. S., Fischer, A., Emmorey, K., and Bellugi, U. (1997). Neural systems mediating American Sign Language: Effects of sensory experience and age of acquisition. Brain and language, 57(3), 285-308.
Olsen, R. K., Pangelinan, M. M., Bogulski, C., Chakravarty, M. M., Luk, G., Grady, C. L., and Bialystok, E. (2015). The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Research, 1612, 128-139.
Ossher, L., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Murphy, K. J., and Troyer, A. K. (2013). The effect of bilingualism on amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68, 8-12.
Paradis, M. (2009). Declarative and Procedural Determinants of Second Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Paradis, M. (2011). Principles underlying the Bilingual Aphasia Test (BAT) and its uses. Clinical Linguistics y Phonetics, 25(6-7), 427-443.
Paradis, M. and Lecours, A. R. (1979). L’aphasie chez les bilingues et les polyglotes. In L’aphasie, eds. A.R. Lecours and F. Lhermitte, (Paris: Flammarion), pp. 605-616.
Parra, M. A., Abrahams, S., Fabi, K., Logie, R., Luzzi, S., and Della Sala, S. (2009). Shortterm memory binding deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Brain, awp036.
Parra, M. A., Abrahams, S., Logie, R. H., Mendez, L. G., Lopera, F., and Della Sala, S. (2010). Visual short-term memory binding deficits in familial Alzheimer’s disease. Brain: a Journal of Neurology, 133(9), 2702-1273.
Perani, D., Dehaene, S., Grassi, F., Cohen, L., Cappa, S. F., Dupoux, E., y otros (1996). Brain processing of native and foreign languages. Neuroreport, 7(15-17), 2439-2444.
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 183-194.
Prior, A., and MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(02), 253-262.
Robins, L.N., Wing, J., Wittchen, H.U., Helzer, J.E., Babor, T.F., Burke, J., y otros (1989). The Composite International Diagnostic Interview: An epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 1069-1077.
Roca, M., Torralva, T., Gleichgerrcht, E., Woolgar, A., Thompson, R., Duncan, J., and Manes, F. (2011). The role of area 10 (BA10) in human multitasking and in social cognition: a lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 49(13), 3525-3531.
Rodriguez‐Fornells, A., De Diego Balaguer, R., and Münte, T. F. (2006). Executive control in bilingual language processing. Language Learning, 56(1), 133-190.
Salvatierra, J. L., and Rosselli, M. (2010). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism.
Sanders, A. E., Hall, C. B., Katz, M. J., and Lipton, R. B. (2012). Non-native language use and risk of incident dementia in the elderly. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 29, 99-108.
Sattler, C., Toro, P., Schönknecht, P., and Schröder, J. (2012). Cognitive activity, education and socioeconomic status as preventive factors for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatry Research, 196(1), 90-95.
Scarmeas, N., and Stern, Y. (2004). Cognitive reserve: implications for diagnosis and prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 4(5), 374-380.
Schweizer, T.A., Ware, J., Fischer, C.E., Craik, F.I., and Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 48, 991-996.
Sheline, Y.I., and Raichle, M.E. (2013). Resting state functional connectivity in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Biological Psychiatry, 74(5), 340-347.
Shiroky, J.S., Schipper, H.M., Bergman, H., Chertkow, H. (2007). Can you have dementia with an MMSE score of 30? American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 22(5), 406-415.
Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., and Baltes, P. B. (1993). Resilience and levels of reserve capacity in later adulthood: Perspectives from life-span theory. Development and Psychopathology, 5(04), 541-566.
Stevenson, J. R., Schroeder, J. P., Nixon, K., Besheer, J., Crews, F. T., and Hodge, C. W. (2009). Abstinence following alcohol drinking produces depression-like behavior and reduced hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(5), 1209-1222.
Stein, M., Federspiel, A., Koenig, T., Wirth, M., Strik, W., Wiest, R., Brandeis, D., and Dierks, T. (2012). Structural plasticity in the language system related to increased second language proficiency. Cortex, 48(4), 458-465.
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015-2028.
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 1006-1012.
Sullivan, G. M., and Feinn, R. (2012). Using effect size-or why the P value is not enough. Journal of Graduate Medical Education, 4(3), 279-282.
Szaflarski, M., Cubbins, L. A., and Ying, J. (2011). Epidemiology of Alcohol Abuse Among US Immigrant Populations. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health / Center for Minority Public Health, 13(4), 647-658.
Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., and Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2.
Tang-Wai, D. F., Knopman, D. S., Geda, Y. E., Edland, S. D., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., y otros (2003). Comparison of the short test of mental status and the mini-mental state examination in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 60(12), 1777-1781.
Tractenberg, R. E., Singer, C. M., and Kaye, J. A. (2006). Characterizing sleep problems in persons with Alzheimer's disease and normal elderly. Journal of Sleep Research, 15(1), 97-103.
Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., Lopez, P., and Manes, F. (2009a). INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): a brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(5), 777-786.
Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., Bekinschtein, T., and Manes, F. (2009b). A neuropsychological battery to detect specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 132(5), 1299-1309.
Ullman, M. T. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4(1), 105-122.
Unger, J. B., Cruz, T. B., Rohrbach, L. A., Ribisl, K. M., Baezconde-Garbanti, L., Chen, X., y otros (2000). English language use as a risk factor for smoking initiation among Hispanic and Asian American adolescents: evidence for mediation by tobacco-related beliefs and social norms. Health Psychology, 19(5), 403.
Valdés, G. and Figueroa, R.A. (1994). Bilingualism and Testing: A Special Case of Bias. Norwood. New Jersey: Ablex Publishing.
Voss, U., and Tuin, I. (2008). Integration of immigrants into a new culture is related to poor sleep quality. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 6(1), 61.
Walker, M.P. and Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory and plasticity. Annual Review of Psychology, 10, 139-166.
Weber-Fox, C. M. and Neville, H. J. (1997). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 231-256.
Weinreich, U. (1953). Languages in Contact: Findings and Problems. La Haya: Mouton.
White, H., Boden-Albala, B., Wang, C., Elkind, M.S., Rundek ,T., Wright, C.B., y otros (2005). Ischemic stroke subtype incidence among whites, blacks, and Hispanics: The Northern Manhattan Study. Circulation, 111, 1327 - 1331.
Woumans, E., Santens, P., Sieben, A., Versijpt, J., Stevens, M., and Duyck, W. (2015). Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(03), 568-574.
Wind, A. W., Schellevis, F. G., Van Staveren, G. E., Scholten, R. J., Jonker, C., and Van Eijk, J. T. (1997). Limitations of the Mini‐Mental State Examination in diagnosing dementia in general practice. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(1), 101-108.
Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M. T., Stern, Y., and Manly, J. J. (2014). Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology, 28(2), 238-246.
Zahuranec, D. B., Brown, D. L., Lisabeth, L. D., Gonzales, N. R., Longwell, P. J., Eden, S. V., y otros (2006). Differences in intracerebral hemorrhage between Mexican Americans and non-Hispanic whites. Neurology, 66(1), 30-34.
Zammit, G., Weiner, J., Damato, N., Sillup, G., McMillan, C. (1999). Quality of life in people with insomnia. Sleep, 22(2), 379-385.
Zhang, J., Goodlett, D. R., and Montine, T. J. (2005). Proteomic biomarker discovery in cerebrospinal fluid for neurodegenerative diseases. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 8(4), 377-386.
Article download
License
In order to support the global exchange of knowledge, the journal Círculo de Lingüística Aplicada a la Comunicación is allowing unrestricted access to its content as from its publication in this electronic edition, and as such it is an open-access journal. The originals published in this journal are the property of the Complutense University of Madrid and any reproduction thereof in full or in part must cite the source. All content is distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 use and distribution licence (CC BY 4.0). This circumstance must be expressly stated in these terms where necessary. You can view the summary and the complete legal text of the licence.